翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Consulate General of the United States, Chengdu
・ Consulate General of the United States, Chennai
・ Consulate General of the United States, Curaçao
・ Consulate General of the United States, Dubai
・ Constructive dilemma
・ Constructive dismissal
・ Constructive empiricism
・ Constructive engagement
・ Constructive eviction
・ Constructive fraud
・ Constructive function theory
・ Constructive heuristic
・ Constructive journalism
・ Constructive non-standard analysis
・ Constructive notice
Constructive perception
・ Constructive possession
・ Constructive Program
・ Constructive proof
・ Constructive quantum field theory
・ Constructive realism
・ Constructive receipt
・ Constructive research
・ Constructive set theory
・ Constructive solid geometry
・ Constructive speech
・ Constructive theology
・ Constructive treason
・ Constructive trust
・ Constructive trusts in English law


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Constructive perception : ウィキペディア英語版
Constructive perception
Constructive perception, is the theory of perception in which the perceiver uses sensory information and other sources of information to construct a cognitive understanding of a stimulus. In contrast to this top-down approach, there is the bottom-up approach of direct perception.
Also known as intelligent perception, constructive perception shows the relationship between intelligence and perception. This comes from the importance of high-order thinking and learning in perception. During perception, hypotheses are formed and tested about percepts that are based on three things: sensory data, knowledge, and high-level cognitive processes. Visual sensations are usually correctly attributed because we unconsciously assimilate information from many sources and then unconsciously make judgments based on this information. The philosophy of Immanuel Kant explains that our perception of the world is reciprocal; it both is affected by and affects our experience of the world.
== Evidence of constructive perception ==
Context effects are not explained by bottom-up theories of accounting. Irving Biederman performed experiments that demonstrated dramatic context effects. For example, Stephen Palmer carried out an experiment in which the participants were asked to identify objects after they were shown either a relevant or irrelevant context. They might be shown a scene of a baseball game, followed by images of a baseball, car, and a phone. The stimuli that was most relevant to the context, the baseball, was recognized quicker than those that were irrelevant, car and phone.
Perceptual constancy gives evidence that high-level constructive processes occur during perception. As lighting conditions change, the color of objects, such as bananas or cherries, appear to remain constant. Even when there is not enough light to even stimulate cone cells and give the sensation of color, bananas and cherries are still perceived as yellow and red, respectively.
Configural-superiority effect is another compelling context effect. This effect is demonstrated by the decrease in response time by participants when identifying objects in complex configurations over objects in isolation. For example, four diagonal lines are shown and participants are asked the location of the odd line indicated by its different orientation. In another condition, an "L" shaped fixed context is added; this creates three triangles and another object made of three lines. Participants were able to discern the odd three line object from the set of triangles quicker than the single diagonal lines alone.

抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Constructive perception」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.